Skip to main content

Age-related changes in factors associated with self-rated health in Swedish older adults– findings from the Gothenburg H70 study cohort born 1930

Abstract

Background

Knowledge about age-related changes in factors associated with self-rated health (SRH) in older adults is still limited.

Objective

To explore changes in SRH and analyze the strength of the association between different factors and SRH at ages 70, 75, 85, and 88 in a cohort born in 1930.

Design

Cross-sectional.

Setting

The Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort Studies.

Subjects

939 individuals, providing 1841 observations.

Method

SRH was assessed using the same question at every examination. Factors potentially affecting SRH included somatic and mental disease burden, functional ability, life satisfaction, and loneliness. Lung function was included as an indicator of physical fitness. Descriptive statistics and binary regression were used to explore cohort characteristics, associated factors, and SRH. GLMM (Generalized linear mixed model) was used to perform a sensitivity analysis and test the robustness of our results.

Results

There was an association between factors and SRH at every age, except feelings of loneliness and having a low disease burden at 85. High disease burden showed the strongest association at 70 and the weakest at 85. Depression showed the strongest association at 85 and the lowest at 88. When also controlling for life satisfaction, the associations changed between the ages, and feelings of loneliness were no longer associated with poor SRH other than at 88. The association between factors and poor SRH was generally stronger at ages 70–75 than at ages 85–88. The sensitivity analysis using GLMM confirmed the robustness of our results.

Conclusion

The proportion of poor SRH decreases with age despite a higher frequency of somatic health conditions. Associated factors varied across ages, indicating that physical factors more strongly influence SRH in younger-old adults, while psychosocial factors have a greater impact on SRH in older-old adults.

Peer Review reports

Background

Self-rated health (SRH) is a global self-assessment of an individual’s health status and is considered a strong predictor of disability, morbidity, and mortality [1,2,3]. With the question, “In general, would you say your health is excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor?” (or some variations thereof), SRH delegates the task of synthesizing the many dimensions that make up the complex concept of health to the individual respondent. SRH allows us to capture elements that more guided questions cannot [4,5,6]. However, our understanding of factors influencing a person to give a poor or excellent health rating is still limited. Jylhä’s [4] well-cited conceptual model describes SRH as the result of a complex evaluation process that considers health-related factors, such as medical diagnoses, functional status, and symptom experience, as well as the contextual frameworks of evaluation, such as age, culture, comparison mechanisms, references, and personal disposition [4]. The model implies that the importance of some evaluation criteria might change with age (i.e., response shift) and that different groups use the response options differently.

Previous studies have identified chronic illness, multimorbidity [1, 7, 8], functional impairment [1, 9], psychological well-being [1, 10], physical activity [11], cognitive function [12], depressive symptoms [13, 14], positive affect [15, 16], life satisfaction [17], as well as demographic factors [4, 18,19,20,21] as significant determinants of SRH in older adults. However, as we age, studies also suggest that the link between symptoms, diagnosed conditions, and functional status, on the one hand, and SRH, on the other hand, changes [8]. There also seems to be a shift in the relative importance of factors affecting SRH, with psychosocial factors becoming more important with age, potentially mitigating the adverse effects of illness and functional decline [22].

When comparing SRH in younger-olds (i.e., 65–75) with older-olds (i.e., > 85 years), studies show that the association between subjective and objective health weakens with advancing age [22,23,24]. Previous research indicates that the gap between objective health and SRH continues to increase with age [25]. This relationship has been identified even in the absolute oldest age groups above age 100 [26]. This paradox is commonly explained by coping skills and decreased health aspiration levels, allowing health to be experienced as satisfactory even if it is worse than before [5, 8, 27, 28]. However, few studies have longitudinally examined individual transitions in SRH of older adults [29], and a closer look at age-related changes in SRH is needed. This study aims to explore changes in SRH and analyze the strength of the association between the different factors and SRH at ages 70, 75, 85, and 88 in a cohort born in 1930.

Methods

Study population

This study is a part of the Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort Studies in Sweden (the H70 studies). The complete study protocol is described elsewhere [30]. All study participants were registered residents in Gothenburg born on pre-selected birth dates. Information regarding date of birth and residential address (both in ordinary and special housing) was obtained from the Swedish Tax Agency’s population registry. In this study, we use data from the 1930 cohort where participants were examined at ages 70 (n = 512), 75 (n = 741), 85 (n = 362), and 88 (n = 226) years (Fig. 1). Due to a loss to follow-up, new participants were added to the 1930 cohort in 2005, 2015, and 2018, resulting in varying numbers of examinations per participant. Some participated in one examination, whereas others participated in all four examination waves. In total, 939 unique participants at ages 70, 75, 85, and 88 years yielded the 1841 observations used in this study (Supplemental Table 1). In the present study, participants with dementia were excluded (n = 252) due to the risk of reporting bias and misinterpretation. The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Gothenburg (approval numbers: 240800/ S227-00, 041104/T453-04, 270415/131 − 15 and 230418/278 − 18).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Sample flow-chart describing the four examination waves. 1The sample consisted of all 70-year-olds born in 1930 as well as participants previously examined in the Gothenburg Women's Study born in 1930. 2 The sarnpleconsisled of participants previously examined at age 70 as well as new 75-years-olds born in 1930 living in Gothenburg (n=384). 3 The sample consisted of participants previously examined at ages 70 and 75 as well as new 85-year-olds bom in 1930 living in Gothenburg (n=67). 4 The sample consisted of participants previously examined at ages 70, 75, and 85 years as well as new 88-year-olds born in 1930 living in Gothenburg (n=14).5 Individuals only participating at one of the examinations

Self-rated health

SRH was assessed using the same question at every examination. At ages 70 and 75, the participants rated their health on a five-point scale (i.e., “excellent,” “very good,” “good,” “moderate,” and “poor”) while at ages 85 and 88, they rated their health on a four-point scale (i.e., “very good,” “good,” “poor,” and “very poor”) with higher values indicating better SRH. To harmonize the response options between examinations, we collapsed “excellent,” “very good,” and “good” into “Good SRH,” and “fair,” “poor,” and “very poor” into “Poor SRH.”

Demographic factors

Educational level was dichotomized as equal to or less than compulsory education (i.e., six years) or more than compulsory (i.e., at least one more year). Marital status was dichotomized as married/cohabiting or living alone.

Somatic health factors

Somatic health conditions were self-reported and ascertained by a positive answer to the question, “Have you ever been told by a doctor that you have…?” In the present study, we included cardiovascular disease (i.e., angina pectoris, myocardial infarction, and intermittent claudication), diabetes (types 1 and 2), and stroke/TIA. Urinary incontinence (yes/no) and joint pain (yes/no) were also included. Lung function was measured with Peak Expiratory Flow (PEF), and a cut-off for normal lung function in older adults was set at > 220 L/minute for women and 350 L/minute for men [31]. The number of conditions was summed into a total score and then categorized into no/minimal disease burden (0–1 somatic health problems), low disease burden (2–3 somatic health problems), and high disease burden (≥ 4 somatic health problems).

Mental health factors

In a psychiatric interview, the participants were asked questions about their mental health, and major depression was identified according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fifth Edition [32], and minor depression according to DSM-IV-TR research criteria [33]. For this study, “any depression” was used to denote those fulfilling the requirements for either major or minor depression.

Functional factors

Activities of daily living (ADL) included feeding, dressing, showering, going to the toilet, getting up from a chair, and indoor and outdoor mobility. Instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) included caring for one’s home. ADL and IADL were coded as either independent (including the use of aids) or dependent (in need of help from another person). The ADL and IADL activities were summed into a total score, and participants with ≥ 1 ADL/IADL dependency were grouped into “any ADL/IADL disability.”

Psychological factors

Level of satisfaction in five life domains (living conditions, social relationships, leisure time, economy, and total life situation) was graded on a 7-point scale and summed to an index score ranging between 5 and 35 points, with higher values indicating a higher level of life satisfaction (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.7 at all four examination points). Feelings of loneliness were rated as either yes or no.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to report sample characteristics and differences in proportions across examination occasions and sexes. Life satisfaction was treated as a continuous variable, and all other variables were categorical. Binary regression analysis was used to test the association between the different factors and SRH. In the first model, the associations were tested separately at each time point, controlling for sex and educational level. In the second model, we also adjusted for life satisfaction. Two-tailed p-values of < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. To test the robustness of the binary regression analysis, we conducted a sensitivity analysis using a Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM). As in the binary regression analysis, we ran two models: the first controlled for sex and educational level, while in the second, life satisfaction was also included to adjust the analysis. The first analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 29.0.00. The sensitivity analysis was performed using RStudio.

Table 1 Characteristics of the sample at each examination (n=1841)

Results

There were 939 individuals in the sample that provided 1841 observations at ages 70, 75, 85, and 88. Participants were examined on either one (n = 370), two (n = 326), three (n = 153), or four (n = 90) occasions (Supplemental Table 1). The characteristics of the participants at each examination are presented in Table 1. There was a difference between examinations regarding all characteristics, except in the proportion of women. The proportion of participants living alone, feeling lonely, having a high disease burden (≥ 4 health problems), and being dependent in ADL/IADL increased with age.

The proportion of participants rating their health as poor differed between the ages, with the highest proportion observed at age 75 (30%) and the lowest at age 88 (15%) (Table 1). At age 75, women more often reported poor SRH (men 25% vs. women 34%, p = 0.015). The original response options used at ages 70 and 75 and at ages 85 and 88, respectively, are presented in Supplemental Table 2. Although statistically significant, the difference between ages in life satisfaction was minimal.

The associations between the different factors and poor SRH, adjusted for sex and educational level, are presented in Table 2. All included factors showed a statistically significant association with poor SRH at every age, except low somatic disease burden at ages 85 and 88 and feeling of loneliness at age 85. At ages 70, 75, and 88, the strongest association with poor SRH was found for high somatic disease burden. At age 85, depression showed the strongest association with poor SRH. ADL/IADL dependency had the second strongest association with poor SRH across all ages.

When comparing the strength of the association between ages, both high and low somatic disease burden showed the strongest association with poor SRH at age 70, high somatic disease burden was weakest at age 85, and low somatic disease burden wasn’t significant at either age 85 or 88. ADL/IADL dependency showed the strongest association with poor SRH at age 70 and the weakest at age 85. Depression showed the strongest association with poor SRH at age 85 and the weakest at age 88. Feeling lonely showed the strongest association with poor SRH at age 88 and the weakest at age 75. Higher life satisfaction decreased the probability of poor SRH at every age, with the highest probability observed at age 70 and the lowest at age 88.

Table 2 The association between factors and poor SRH at different ages, controlling for sex and educational level

In the first model, life satisfaction reduced the probability of poor SRH at all ages. Life satisfaction was then entered as a control factor in the second model to test its protective effect on SRH. When also controlling for life satisfaction, the associations between factors and poor SRH changed between the ages, and feelings of loneliness were no longer associated with poor SRH other than at age 88 (Table 3). The association between depression and poor SRH remained strongest at age 85 despite the association decreasing at all ages. The association between low somatic disease burden and poor SHR decreased at ages 70 and 75 and between high disease burden and poor SRH at ages 70 and 75. However, the association between high disease burden and poor SRH increased at age 85, while the association with ADL/IADL dependency decreased at all ages. The associations between the different factors and poor SRH, adjusted for sex, educational level, and life satisfaction, are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 The association between factors and poor SRH at different ages, controlling for sex, educational level, and life satisfaction

Sensitivity analysis

To assess the robustness of our results, we conducted a sensitivity analysis using a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM). This approach allowed us to account for potential within-subject correlations by including participant ID as a random intercept, thereby controlling for intra-individual variability across repeated observations.

As in the binary regressions, we ran two models: the first model adjusted for sex and educational level, and the second model was also adjusted for life satisfaction. The results from the GLMM analysis, presented in Supplement Table 3, were generally consistent with the initial logistic regression findings. However, the inclusion of interaction terms in the GLMM provided additional insight into age-related variations. Compared to the reference group at age 70, the associations between SRH and health-related factors generally strengthened at age 75, suggesting a peak in these relationships. However, by the ages of 85 and 88, the associations weakened or were no longer statistically significant, indicating a possible age-related decline in their relevance. In line with our initial analysis, the association between poor SRH and high somatic disease burden was reduced at ages 85 and 88, compared to younger age groups.

In the binary regression, loneliness showed a stronger association with poor SRH at age 88 compared to age 70. However, in the GLMM, the overall association between loneliness and SRH was not significant, and no clear age-related interaction was observed. The GLMM analysis, which included adjustments for sex, educational level, and life satisfaction (Supplement Table 4), confirmed the overall trend observed in the binary regression: associations between SRH and health-related factors generally weakened when life satisfaction was added in the analysis.

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to explore changes in SRH and analyze the strength of the association between the different factors and SRH at ages 70, 75, 85, and 88 in 939 individuals, providing 1841 observations. The results showed that somatic disease burden, being ADL/IADL dependent, living alone, and feeling lonely increased with age, although the proportion with depression was largest at age 75. Only 15% reported poor SRH in the oldest age group (88 years), but as many as 30% at age 75. Although SRH, somatic, and mental health fluctuated over time, the level of life satisfaction remained unchanged.

While the participants rated their SRH better at age 88 than 75, there was an age-related increase in several factors used in this study. It is possible that resilience, successful coping, and acceptance of one’s current health and functional status made the surviving participants more health-positive with increasing age [29, 34]. Resilience is crucial in how individuals adapt to and cope with chronic health conditions, enabling them to manage challenges more effectively [35]. Higher levels of resilience might help mitigate the risk of functional disabilities and buffer the impact of new health issues [36]. Previous studies have shown that SRH remains stable over time in older ages [37, 38]. Despite higher levels of disease burden, older-olds tend to rate their health as good, perhaps due to successful adaptation strategies enabling them to cope with their illnesses and functional disability [8, 39, 40]. In our study, only 15% of the 88-year-olds rated their health as poor compared to 30% of the 75-year-olds.

Consistent with previous studies [22, 23], our results showed that controlling for sex and educational level, the association between factors and SRH shifted between the ages. At ages 70, 75, and 88, high somatic disease burden showed the strongest association with poor SRH. In contrast, at age 85, depression showed the strongest association with poor SRH. Although our results indicate a shift between somatic and mental factors, being dependent on others in everyday life showed the second strongest association with poor SRH at all ages, especially at ages 85 and 88. Autonomy and the ability to function independently are essential to a person’s sense of control, self-efficacy, and self-determination, and previous studies have shown a relationship between being able to manage independently in everyday life and a sense of well-being in older adults [41, 42].

Our results showed that a high level of life satisfaction was associated with better SRH at all ages, although the proportion of participants with a high disease burden and functional disability increased with age. In contrast to previous studies showing that the association between life satisfaction and SRH increases with age [22], our results indicated that life satisfaction remained essentially unchanged between ages. When controlling for life satisfaction, poor SRH was no longer associated with feelings of loneliness, other than at age 88, and the association with depression declined at all ages. Also, the association between somatic disease burden and ADL/IADL dependency decreased. Both SRH and life satisfaction reflect the individual’s reflective judgment, and a possible explanation as to why controlling for life satisfaction affected the associations between factors and SRH might be that life satisfaction and SRH overlap. Previous studies have shown a mutual mediating relationship between SRH and life satisfaction, where good SRH indicates a higher life satisfaction score and vice versa [43, 44].

Quite notably, when controlling for life satisfaction, the association between functional disability and poor SRH at age 88 disappeared. Although there might be several explanations for this finding, it indicates that psychosocial factors become more important with age. Older adults who experience high levels of contentment in areas indirectly related to health, such as social relationships (e.g., marital status, children or friends), living conditions, lifestyle, and income, are possibly better equipped to cope with both current health conditions and upcoming health problems [45, 46].

The sensitivity analysis, conducted using the Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM), confirmed the robustness of our findings. While individual and age-related variability may influence specific estimates, the overall conclusions remained consistent. Compared to the binary regression, the GLMM provides a more nuanced understanding by accounting for intra-individual variability. This allowed us to identify a general decline in the strength of associations between health-related factors and self-rated health (SRH) with advancing age. Poor SRH remained strongly associated with somatic disease burden, functional ability, and depression across models. The observed age-related pattern is consistent with the binary regression results, which also showed stronger associations at age 75 and weaker or non-significant associations at ages 85 and 88. However, the GLMM more clearly illustrates how intra-individual variability contributes to this attenuation across age groups.

Taken together, these findings support the validity of SRH as a broad indicator of age-related health, even though its associations with some health-related factors appear to weaken in the oldest age groups.

This study has both strengths and limitations. The strengths of this study include the four-time points of cross-sectional health examinations over an 18-year follow-up period in a population-based sample of older. There are also several limitations. Firstly, due to study attrition, new participants were added to the sample at ages 75, 85, and 88, affecting the possibility of analyzing the data longitudinally. Secondly, the data used in this study is mainly cross-sectional, only allowing us to explore inter-individual changes in the association between related factors and SRH. Thirdly, the risk of selection bias increases by primarily using cross-sectional data. For instance, at age 75, new participants were included, which could lead to skewed data. Another potential risk for selection bias lies in the likelihood that participants with poor SRH either die or decline participation due to their health status, leading to a healthier sample as age increases. Fourthly, the total number of somatic health conditions and ADL/IADL dependencies were positively skewed and, therefore, categorized. If the total scores had been normally distributed, it is possible that more detailed information about the variation of values and their association with SRH could have been captured. However, it is also possible that the categorization enabled us to detect relationships that might otherwise have been missed. Fifthly, although we controlled for sex, education, and life satisfaction, group differences might affect the strength of the association between related factors and SRH. For instance, Spuling et al. [22] suggest that the association between SRH and various factors is affected not only by age but also by birth year, pointing out that different birth cohorts rate their health differently. Since we only included one birth cohort, we cannot analyze the effect of birth cohorts in this study. Sixthly, the proportion of those who rated their health as poor was two times larger at age 75 compared to age 88. A possible explanation for this difference is survival. Although the somatic disease burden and ADL/IADL dependency increased with age, there is a possibility that those examined at age 75 had a higher mortality rate than those examined at age 88, meaning that those still alive and in sufficient health to the participants in the study at age 88 was those in better health age 75. Seventhly, this study explored the association between mainly self-rated associated factors and SRH. Participants with dementia were excluded from this study due to the potential limitations of self-reported data in this population. As the disease progresses, individuals may experience significant difficulties in recalling past events or understanding the context of questions posed to them.

Lastly, although the SRH question was phrased identically between examinations, the response options varied over time. To compare SRH between ages, we collapsed the response options into two categories, potentially limiting the detection of more nuanced changes in SRH between examinations.

Conclusion

SRH is a frequently used health measurement that evaluates individuals’ subjective health. The knowledge about SRH in later life and factors associated with individuals’ health assessments is limited, which motivates our research. The results show that the proportion of poor SRH decreases with age, despite an increase in somatic health disorders. We also identify that the associated factors varied in significance across ages, suggesting that physical factors have a more prominent role in SRH among younger-olds, while psychosocial factors are more important among older-olds. When accounting for intra-individual variability, the results indicate that the association between health-related factors and poor SRH becomes weaker with age. However, additional longitudinal research is needed to gain a deeper understanding of the changes in related factors that influence SRH changes with increasing age, and thereby identify changes over the lifespan.

Data availability

The data used in this article can be made available by the corresponding author upon request.

Abbreviations

ADL:

Activities of daily living

IADL:

Instrumental activities of daily living (IADL)

GLMM:

Generalized Linear Mixed Model

SRH:

Self-rated health

The H70 studies:

The Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort Studies in Sweden

References

  1. DeSalvo KB, Bloser N, Reynolds K, He J, Muntner P. Mortality prediction with a single general self-rated health question. A meta-analysis. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21(3):267–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Falk H, Skoog I, Johansson L, Guerchet M, Mayston R, Hörder H, et al. Self-rated health and its association with mortality in older adults in China, India and Latin America-a 10/66 dementia research group study. Age Ageing. 2017;46(6):932–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Dramé M, Cantegrit E, Godaert L. Self-Rated health as a predictor of mortality in older adults: A systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2023;20(5).

  4. Jylha M. What is self-rated health and why does it predict mortality? Towards a unified conceptual model. Soc Sci Med. 2009;69(3):307–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. J Health Soc Behav. 1997;38(1):21–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Lisko I, Törmäkangas T, Jylhä M. Structure of self-rated health among the oldest old: analyses in the total population and those living with dementia. SSM - Popul Health. 2020;11:100567.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Galenkamp H, Braam AW, Huisman M, Deeg DJH. Somatic Multimorbidity and self-rated health in the older population. Journals Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Social Sci. 2011;66(3):380–6.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Galenkamp H, Braam AW, Huisman M, Deeg DJ. Seventeen-year time trend in poor self-rated health in older adults: changing contributions of chronic diseases and disability. Eur J Public Health. 2013;23(3):511–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Straatmann VS, Vetrano DL, Fratiglioni L, Calderón-Larrañaga A. Disease or function? What matters most for self-rated health in older people depends on age. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2020;32(8):1591–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Zhang L, Bi X, Ding Z. Health lifestyles and Chinese oldest-old’s subjective well-being—evidence from a latent class analysis. BMC Geriatr. 2021;21(1):206.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Darviri C, Artemiadis AK, Tigani X, Alexopoulos EC. Lifestyle and self-rated health: a cross-sectional study of 3,601 citizens of Athens, Greece. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):619.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Golini N, Egidi V. The latent dimensions of poor Self-Rated health: how chronic diseases, functional and emotional dimensions interact influencing Self-Rated health in Italian elderly. Soc Indic Res. 2016;128(1):321–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Schnittker J. When mental health becomes health: age and the shifting meaning of self-evaluations of general health. Milbank Q. 2005;83(3):397–423.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Peleg S, Nudelman G. Associations between self-rated health and depressive symptoms among older adults: does age matter? Soc Sci Med. 2021;280:114024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Benyamini Y, Leventhal EA, Leventhal H. Gender differences in processing information for making self-assessments of health. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(3):354–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Peersman W, Cambier D, De Maeseneer J, Willems S. Gender, educational and age differences in meanings that underlie global self-rated health. Int J Public Health. 2012;57(3):513–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Siahpush M, Spittal M, Singh GK. Happiness and life satisfaction prospectively predict self-rated health, physical health, and the presence of limiting, long-term health conditions. Am J Health Promot. 2008;23(1):18–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Nummela O, Seppänen M, Uutela A. The effect of loneliness and change in loneliness on self-rated health (SRH): a longitudinal study among aging people. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2011;53(2):163–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Verropoulou G. Key elements composing self-rated health in older adults: a comparative study of 11 European countries. Eur J Ageing. 2009;6(3):213–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Jylhä M, Volpato S, Guralnik JM. Self-rated health showed a graded association with frequently used biomarkers in a large population sample. J Clin Epidemiol. 2006;59(5):465–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Byun M, Kim E, Ahn H. Factors contributing to poor self-rated health in older adults with lower income. Healthc (Basel). 2021;9(11):1515.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Spuling SM, Wurm S, Tesch-Römer C, Huxhold O. Changing predictors of Self-Rated health: disentangling age and cohort effects. Psychol Aging. 2015;30(2):462–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. French DJ, Sargent-Cox K, Luszcz MA. Correlates of subjective health across the aging lifespan: Understanding Self-Rated health in the oldest old. J Aging Health. 2012;24(8):1449–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Pinquart M. Correlates of subjective health in older adults: A Meta-Analysis. Psychol Aging. 2001;16(3):414–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Henchoz K, Cavalli S, Girardin M. Health perception and health status in advanced old age: A paradox of association. J Aging Stud. 2008;22(3):282–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Araújo L, Teixeira L, Ribeiro O, Paúl C. Objective vs. subjective health in very advanced ages: looking for discordance in centenarians. Front Med. 2018;5:189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Tornstam L. Health and Self-Perception: A systems theoretical approach. Gerontologist. 1975;15(3):264–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum Relations (New York). 1954;7(2):117–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Leinonen R, Heikkinen E, Jylhä M. Predictors of decline in self-assessments of health among older people — a 5-year longitudinal study. Soc Sci Med. 2001;52(9):1329–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Rydberg Sterner T, Ahlner F, Blennow K, Dahlin-Ivanoff S, Falk H, Havstam Johansson L, et al. The Gothenburg H70 birth cohort study 2014-16: design, methods and study population. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019;34(2):191–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Magave JA, Bezerra SJS, Matos AP, Pinto A, Pegorari MS, Ohara DG. Peak expiratory flow as an index of frailty syndrome in older adults: A Cross-Sectional study. J Nutr Health Aging. 2020;24(9):993–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington,DC:: American Psychiatric Association;; 2013.

  33. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Hildon Z, Smith G, Netuveli G, Blane D. Understanding adversity and resilience at older ages. Sociol Health Illn. 2008;30(5):726–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Mertens V-C, Bosma H, Groffen DAI, van Eijk JTM. Good friends, high income or resilience? What matters most for elderly patients? Eur J Pub Health. 2012;22(5):666–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Manning LK, Carr DC, Kail BL. Do higher levels of resilience buffer the deleterious impact of chronic illness on disability in later life?? Gerontologist. 2016;56(3):514–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Almevall A, Almevall AD, Öhlin J, Gustafson Y, Zingmark K, Niklasson J, et al. Self-rated health in old age, related factors and survival: A 20-Year longitudinal study within the Silver-MONICA cohort. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2024;122:105392.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Feenstra M, van Munster BC, MacNeil Vroomen JL, de Rooij SE, Smidt N. Trajectories of self-rated health in an older general population and their determinants: the lifelines cohort study. BMJ Open. 2020;10(2):e035012–e.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Leinonen R, Heikkinen E, Jylhä M. Self-rated health and self-assessed change in health in elderly men and women–a five-year longitudinal study. Soc Sci Med. 1998;46(4–5):591–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Simonsson B, Molarius A. Self-rated health and associated factors among the oldest-old: results from a cross-sectional study in Sweden. Arch Public Health. 2020;78:6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Covinsky KE, Palmer RM, Fortinsky RH, Counsell SR, Stewart AL, Kresevic D, et al. Loss of independence in activities of daily living in older adults hospitalized with medical illnesses: increased vulnerability with age. J Am Geriatr Soc (JAGS). 2003;51(4):451–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Sixsmith J, Sixsmith A, Fänge AM, Naumann D, Kucsera C, Tomsone S, et al. Healthy ageing and home: the perspectives of very old people in five European countries. Soc Sci Med. 2014;106:1–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kim ES, Delaney SW, Tay L, Chen Y, Diener ED, Vanderweele TJ. Life satisfaction and subsequent physical, behavioral, and psychosocial health in older adults. Milbank Q. 2021;99(1):209–39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Qazi SL, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Rikkonen T, Sund R, Kröger H, Isanejad M, Sirola J. Physical capacity, subjective health, and life satisfaction in older women: a 10-year follow-up study. BMC Geriatr. 2021;21(1):658.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Pinquart M, Sörensen S. Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on subjective Well-Being in later life: A Meta-Analysis. Psychol Aging. 2000;15(2):187–224.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Asante S, Karikari G. Social relationships and the health of older adults: an examination of social connectedness and perceived social support. J Ageing Longev. 2022;2(1):49–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participants of the Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort Study, your contributions have been most valuable.

Funding

Open access funding provided by University of Gothenburg.

The study was financed by grants from the Swedish state under the agreement between the Swedish government and the county councils, the ALF-agreement (ALF965812, ALF 716681), Stena Foundation, Swedish Research Council (11267, 2005–8460, 2007–7462, 2012–5041, 2015–02830, 2019 − 01096, 2022 − 00882, 2013–8717, NEAR 2017 − 00639), Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Wellfare (2004 − 0145, 2006 − 0596, 2008 − 1111, 2010 − 0870, 2013 − 1202, 2018 − 00471, 2001–2646, 2003 − 0234, 2004 − 0150, 2006-0020, 2008 − 1229, 2012 − 1138, AGECAP 2013–2300, 2013–2496), Konung Gustaf V: s och Drottning Victorias Frimurarestiftelse, Hjärnfonden (FO2014-0207, FO2016-0214, FO2018-0214, FO2019-0163, FO2020-0235, FO2024-0341-HK-76), Alzheimerfonden (AF-554461, AF-647651, AF-743701, AF-844671, AF-930868, AF-940139, AF-968441, AF-980935), Eivind och Elsa K: son Sylvans stiftelse, The Alzheimer’s Association Zenith Award (ZEN-01-3151), The Alzheimer’s Association Stephanie B. Overstreet Scholars (IIRG-00-2159), The Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation, Stiftelsen Söderström-Königska Sjukhemmet, Stiftelsen för Gamla Tjänarinnor, Handlanden Hjalmar Svenssons Forskningsfond.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KT, HFE, and LJ were responsible for the design and conceptualization of the study. KT conducted the analysis and data interpretation. KT and HFE drafted the manuscript and tables. KT, HFE, LJ, MS, AIB, and IS contributed all with critical manuscript revision and provided further contributions and suggestions.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karolina Thörnqvist.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All methods were conducted in accordance with ethical standards and regulations. The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Gothenburg (approval numbers: 240800/ S227-00, 041104/T453-04, 270415/131 − 15 and 230418/278 − 18). This study was carried out in adherence to the Declaration of Helsinki. Before taking part in the H70 study, the participants provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

None.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Thörnqvist, K., Johansson, L., Solevid, M. et al. Age-related changes in factors associated with self-rated health in Swedish older adults– findings from the Gothenburg H70 study cohort born 1930. BMC Geriatr 25, 275 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12877-025-05923-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12877-025-05923-8

Keywords